Here's a summary
of the Western media discussion of what motivated U. S. Staff Sgt.
Robert Bales to allegedly kill 16 Afghans, including 9 children: he
was drunk, he was experiencing financial stress, he was passed over
for a promotion, he had a traumatic brain injury, he had marital
problems, he suffered from the stresses of four tours of duty, he
“saw his buddy's leg blown off the day before the massacre,” etc.
Here's a summary
of the Western media discussion of what motivates Muslims to kill
Americans: they are primitive, fanatically religious, hateful
terrorists.
Greenwald also points out that when the
media try to understand possible causes of the bad actions of
American soldiers, we don't confuse them with excuses for their
actions. But when anyone suggests that Afghans who commit crimes may
be responding to the illegal and increasingly violent occupation of
their country, they are immediately accused of attempting to justify
or defend terrorism.
This illustrates what social
psychologist Thomas Pettigrew called the ultimate attribution error. Attribution refers to the process of identifying the
causes of events. Humans are known to make a number of systematic
errors when assigning causality. The ultimate attribution error
refers to a systematic bias in the way we analyze the behavior of
ingroups vs. outgroups, or friends vs. enemies. Good and successful
actions by ingroup members are attributed to their stable internal
dispositions, such as ability or motivation. But our own and our
friends' bad or unsuccessful behavior is seen as a temporary product
of a difficult situation or environment.
When analyzing the behavior
of an outgroup, especially a disliked one, this tendency is reversed.
Their bad behavior is seen as a product of their evil nature, while
their good behavior is a product of a temporary, favorable, external
situation. (Think of the tendency of prejudiced whites to attribute
black success to affirmative action.) Needless to say, any
unfavorable stereotypes we have of outgroup members will be
maintained by these attributions, even in the face of behavior that
might appear to contradict them.
Greenwald's point reminded me of a pair of related studies by Michael Morris and Kaiping Peng. In the first,
they analyzed newspaper accounts in the New York Times
and the Chinese-language World Journal
of two mass murders that occurred in the United States, one committed
by a Chinese man and the other by an American man. Each unit, or
clause, that gave an explanation for the crime was classified
according to whether it referred to a permanent disposition of the
actor or a temporary situational influence.
The
Times article showed
the ultimate attribution error. American reporters attributed more
causality to situational factors when describing the American
murderer than the Chinese murderer. On the other hand, more
dispositional traits were attributed to the Chinese than to the
American killer. The Chinese-language newspaper did not show this
bias.
The
second study was an experiment. Chinese and American participants
were given an article about either the American or the Chinese
murderer to read. The articles were balanced in that they referred
to an equal number of personal and situational causes of the crime.
Afterwards, the participants were asked to rate the importance of the
personal and situational explanations mentioned to in the article.
As predicted, the American participants gave more weight to
dispositional factors when rating the Chinese than the American
murderer, and more weight to situational factors when rating the
American than the Chinese killer. The Chinese participants did not
show this bias.
This
is a relatively conservative test of the ultimate attribution error,
since there was no particular reason to think the American
participants were hostile to individual Chinese citizens. In this
study, the Chinese are an outgroup rather than an enemy. You would
expect the contrast to be greater when we are at war with the
outgroup.
The
study shows that the ultimate attribution error occurs both in our
individual judgments and in the mass media, but it doesn't address
the chicken vs. egg problem. Do the media influence our judgments,
do they pander to what they know are our preferences, or both?
Finally,
you'll notice that in this study the ultimate attribution error is
uniquely characteristic of Americans. I hope to discuss how and why
Americans' attributions differ from those of most of the rest of the
world's people in a future post.
No comments:
Post a Comment
Comments are always welcome.