Photo by itwuzcryptic |
In a soon-to-be-published paper,
Jessica Witt and James Brockmole of Notre Dame University report five
studies in which they asked student participants to look at slides of
men carrying either guns or harmless objects. The students had about
a half-second to indicate whether the person was armed or not by
raising or lowering a lever. The experiment varied the handle of
that lever. In one condition, it was shaped like a realistic-looking
toy handgun. It the other, it was a rubber ball. The results showed
that the participants were more likely to “see” the person in the
slide as holding a gun when they themselves were holding a gun than
when they were holding a ball.
The authors interpret these findings as
consistent with the theory of event coding. This theory is a
scientific variation on the old saying that when you're holding a
hammer, everything looks like a nail. When planning an action with
an object or tool, you are predisposed to identify other objects on
which the tool might be used.
The authors also considered the theory
of conceptual priming. This theory suggests that the mere presence
of a weapon in the environment predisposes people to “see” other
weapons. They rule this explanation out due to one of their five
experiments. In this variation, a real firearm was placed on the
table while the subjects were performing the task using the lever
with the rubber ball handle. There was no greater tendency to see
the man in the slide as holding a firearm when a real gun was present
in their environment than when it was not.
I suspect that they ruled out the priming explanation prematurely. First of all, there are other
studies showing that aggressive cues in the environment affect
behavior. For example, in a study by Berkowitz and LePage,
frustrated participants were more likely to respond aggressively when
there were weapons (a handgun and a rifle) on the table than when
there was either a neutral object (a badminton racquet) or nothing on
the table. Secondly, in the Witt and Brockmole study, participants
were exposed to many photos of handguns, and guns were referred to when explaining the task. It's possible that
in such a gun-saturated environment, the presence of yet another
gun—albeit a real one—didn't make much difference.
I go into these details not only
because I believe in priming—an effect supported by literally
thousands of studies. It also affects the practical implications of Witt and Brockmole studies. A strict interpretation of them would state that the
bias to “see” a gun occurs only when you are holding a gun in
your hand.
In this country, all but eight states
permit ordinary people to openly carry a firearm either on their
person or in their car (although some of them require a license), and
all but Illinois (and D. C.) allow people to carry concealed weapons.
This research implies that carrying a firearm makes you more likely
to see others as dangerous. Does it matter whether you are carrying
the gun in your hand, in a holster, or in the glove compartment of
your car? Probably. You are more likely to be aware of the gun when
it's in your hand (as Zimmerman's apparently was). But I suspect
that carrying a concealed weapon alters threat perception as well.
Maybe future studies will test the limits of this effect.
No comments:
Post a Comment
Comments are always welcome.