Tuesday, October 29, 2013

UPMC Gives Pittsburgh the One-Finger Salute--Twice

UPMC, the largest member of Pittsburgh's health care oligopoly, whose fees are well above the national average, was in the news twice last week. The City of Pittsburgh is suing UPMC, which claims to be a non-profit, to strip it of its tax-exempt status. In a court hearing last week, UPMC claimed it does not owe any payroll taxes because it does not have any employees! (Although their website claims they have 55,000 employees, UPMC says these people are employed by subsidiaries.) So far, Judge R. Stanton Wettick, Jr., has not ruled on the credibility of this claim.

The Bombardier BD-700-1A10 Global Express
On Friday, it was learned that UPMC is spending $51 million on a new corporate jet plane, a Bombardier Global Express, described as “a luxury, ultra-long range business jet with twin Rolls Royce engines.” For security reasons, the flight plans of this airplane are to remain hidden from the public.

I'm sure Pittsburghers who are struggling to pay their medical bills will be thrilled to hear that UPMC is able to afford $51 million for a new stealth jetliner. But who is going to ride around on this luxury aircraft, since, as we now know, UPMC has no employees?

You may also be interested in reading:


Monday, October 28, 2013

Lou Reed (1942-2013)


We caught Lou Reed at the Radio City Music Hall around the time his New York album came out (1989). His guest was Little Jimmy Scott. Here's everyone's favorite side from that album:



In case you didn't catch the last verse:

          Well Americans don't care much for anything,
          land and water the least
          And animal life is so low on the totem pole
          with human life not worth more than infected yeast

          Americans don't care too much for beauty
          They'll shit in a river, dump battery acid in a stream
          They'll watch dead rats wash up on a beach
          and complain if they can't swim

          They say things are done for the majority
          Don't believe half of what you see and none of what you hear
          It's like my painter friend Donald said to me
          “Stick a fork in their ass and turn them over, they're done”

Sunday, October 27, 2013

Just Asking

The following letter appeared in yesterday's Pittsburgh Post-Gazette.

           Under attack?

In this day and age, we hear and read on an almost weekly basis, instances of hackers penetrating many government and industry websites to install malicious malware and viruses. So I'm kind of surprised to see no one inquiring whether the Affordable Care Act website has likewise been attacked by individuals intent on installing similar defects in the website software to negatively impact its successful rollout, and to further embarrass and smear the president. Just asking.

Steve Siskind
Canonsburg

I have been wondering the same thing myself. I can understand why, if this is true, the Obama administration might not want it to be known. But shouldn't the news media be inquiring about this?

Friday, October 25, 2013

The Coverage Gap: The Real Failure of Obamacare

Right now, the corporate media are focused like a laser on the “scandal” of the malfunctioning of the national health insurance exchange website. Although these glitches reflect negatively on the Obama administration, they are technical problems that will be fixed. They have nothing to do with the substance of the law. A more important failure is the number of people, particularly poor people, who will remain uninsured after the Affordable Care Act (ACA) is implemented. This is not the fault of the act as written, but is the result of the Supreme Court decision on its constitutionality and intractable opposition from Republican politicians.

An October study by the Kaiser Family Foundation (KFF) estimates that 5.2 million non-elderly adults living below the federal poverty level (FPL) will remain uninsured in 2014 because they live in the 26 states that—at the time of the study—had declined to participate in the Medicaid expansion. These states are concentrated in the South and West and are controlled largely by Republican governors and legislatures. About half of Americans, but 58% of America's uninsured working poor, live in those states. Pennsylvania is among them. Our Medicaid expansion status is uncertain, but even if the Corbett administration reaches agreement with the federal government on an expansion plan, it is unlikely to be implemented in 2014.

To review, of the approximately 30 million people who were expected to be insured for the first time under the ACA, fully half of them—the poorest half—were to be insured through Medicaid expansion. Traditional Medicaid is jointly administered by the state and federal governments. Federal law requires that all children be covered if their family makes less than the FPL. Children under six are covered up to 133% of FPL. The eligibility rules for adults are determined by the states. In most states, adults without children don't qualify no matter how poor they are. The income level at which parents with dependent children qualify for Medicaid varies from a low of about 20% of FPL in the least generous states to a high of 133% is the most generous states. (In Pennsylvania, it is 46%.)

The ACA expanded Medicaid by making everyone—children and adults—eligible for Medicaid if their family income is 138% of FPL or less. Since this is expensive, the feds agreed to pay most of the cost: 100% in 2014, dropping to 95% in 2017 and 90% in 2020. The ACA required states to implement Medicaid expansion. If they refused, the federal government threatened to withhold its contribution to traditional Medicare—about 57% of the cost. However, this clashed with the conservative majority of the Supreme Court's long-term goal of rolling back federal regulation of the states. In National Federation of Independent Business v. Sibelius, the Supremes decided that the Medicaid expansion rules were coercive and that states may opt out. This denies medical care to many of the Americans who need it most, people who fall into the coverage gap.

The ACA provides subsidies, on a sliding scale, for people individuals and families whose income is between 100% and 400% of FPL. The coverage gap consists of those people, living in states that do not expand Medicaid, who are not poor enough to qualify for Medicaid in their state, but whose income is below 100% of FPL, the level at which the subsidies kick in. This is illustrated in the chart below.

As noted, poor and uninsured Americans tend to be concentrated in the “red states” that are not expanding Medicaid. Twenty percent of the people in the coverage gap live in Texas, and another 15% live in Florida, followed by Georgia with 8% and North Carolina with 6%. At present, 6.8% of the residents of states that are participating Medicaid expansion are poor and uninsured, but 9.1% of the residents of the refusing states are poor and uninsured. The chart below shows the breakdown of the poor and uninsured by race, and shows that Medicaid expansion has a discriminatory impact.
Live in States Expanding
Live in States Not Expanding
White
40%
60%
Black
32%
68%
Hispanic
51%
49%
Asian
70%
30%
Total
42%
58%
These are America's working poor. By occupation, the folks most likely to be poor and uninsured are (1) cashiers, (2) construction laborers, (3) housekeepers, (4) cooks, and (5) waiters and waitresses.

At the time the ACA was passed, single payer advocates noted that the ACA provided less than universal coverage. The largest excluded group is undocumented immigrants, but the ACA also excludes native Americans and people who are incarcerated, have a religious objection, or can prove financial hardship. To that we must now add 5.2 million working poor Americans, a target group that the ACA was clearly intended to help. These people are being left to die for lack of health care. It will be interesting to see whether they respond at the ballot box when they realize what their state politicians have done to them.

Single payer health insurance is needed now more than ever.

You may also be interested in reading:



Friday, October 18, 2013

Is Democracy Possible? Appendix

To make sense of this post, you must first read Part 1 and Part 2.

Kahan and his colleagues did not discuss differences between the responses of liberals and conservatives to his four contingency problems, in spite of the fact that political ideology interacted with his other variables in a way that was statistically significant.

The asymmetry thesis, most strongly identified with journalist Chris Mooney, states that politically motivated reasoning is greater among conservatives than among liberals. Social psychologist John Jost and his colleagues present evidence that conservatism is more strongly correlated with measures of dogmatism and resistance to change than liberalism.

There is evidence of ideological asymmetry in the Kahan experiment. Here it is:

Table 5
These curves represent the distributions of the responses in each condition. The high point of each distribution is the mean. To help us keep these findings straight, Kahan has drawn the liberals in blue and the conservatives in red. The top two figures are of little interest. They merely show that liberals and conservatives are equally likely to answer the questions about the skin cream incorrectly.

However, the bottom two graphs show ideological polarization. Specifically, the conservatives are more likely than the liberals to answer the question correctly when the correct answer corresponds with their expectations, and they are more likely than the liberals to answer incorrectly when the correct answer conflict with their ideology. This is true of both the low numeracy participants (on the left) and the high numeracy participants (on the right). Stated simply, the two red distributions are farther apart than the two blue ones.

Of course, since this study involves only a single issue, another way of interpreting these data is that conservatives care more about gun control than liberals do.

Table 6
You can also see this ideological asymmetry in the bottom figure of Table 3, repeated above, where, at most levels of numeracy, the two red lines are farther apart than the two blue lines. In a blog post, Kahan acknowledges this evidence of asymmetry, but contends that it less interesting than the fact that both liberals and conservatives show confirmatory bias at high levels of numeracy. He points out that the greatest evidence of asymmetry occurs at low to moderate levels of numeracy (inside the solid circle). At the very highest levels of numeracy (inside the broken circle), the two conservative groups appear to be converging, while the two liberal groups are diverging.

He's right about that. Committed liberals must acknowledge the disappointing performance of the highly numerate liberals. The only reasonable explanation is that bright, highly educated liberals are biased too. But that doesn't change the fact that, in this experiment, conservatives show more evidence of ideological bias than liberals do.

You may also be interested in reading:


Wednesday, October 16, 2013

Is Democracy Possible? Part 2

You must read Part 1 before continuing or none of this will make sense.

A study was conducted by Dan Kahan of Yale University and three colleagues involving a demographically representative sample of 1111 American adults recruited by Polimetrix/YouGov, an online survey research firm. Preliminary questions were used to categorize the participants along two dimensions:
  • Liberalism v. Conservatism: They rated themselves as very liberal to very conservative on a 5-point scale, and strong Democrat to strong Republican on a 7-point scale. These scales were combined, and the participants were divided at the midpoint into liberal Democrats and conservative Republicans.
  • Numeracy: Participants' numeracy was measured using nine real world mathematical problems. They received a numeracy score of 0 to 9.

Each participant was then asked to answer one of the four judgment of contingency problems shown in Table 2 of Part 1. These are labeled by the correct answer to the problem:
  • Rash increases: The rash got worse when the skin cream was used.
  • Rash decreases: The rash got better when the skin cream was used.
  • Crime increases: There was more crime with the ban on concealed weapons.
  • Crime decreases: The ban on concealed weapons reduced crime.
Table 3
The top graph presents the results for the skin cream questions. On the vertical dimension, the higher the line, the more people answered the question correctly. Horizontally, as you go from left to right, numeracy increases. The four groups all show almost the same results. As you would expect with content-neutral problems, as numeracy increases, more people answer correctly.

The bottom graph shows the results for the gun control questions. There is a tendency for scores to improve with numeracy, but the results are not uniform. The most striking trend is evidence of confirmatory bias. Participants are more likely to give the correct answer in the two conditions where it is consistent with their political views—the liberals when crime decreases and the conservatives when crime increases. When the correct conclusion is inconsistent with the participant's partisan ideology, the lines are almost flat, which means that the high numeracy people are not successfully correcting their biases. The numerate liberals claim that gun control is effective, even when the evidence shows it is not. The numerate conservatives claim that concealed weapons discourage crime, even when the evidence shows they do not. These labels may help to make the graph more clear.

Table 4
What is going on in the minds of these participants? It's likely that participants in all four groups initially engage in System 1 thinking. They look at the data and immediately believe what their political ideology leads them to expect. For the two groups in which the data are consistent with their prior beliefs, this is no problem. If they use System 2 to do the math, they will become more confident of the correct conclusion. But the two groups in which the data are inconsistent with their prior beliefs are faced with a conflict if they engage in System 2 thought. The results suggest that either they don't bother, or if they do, they manage to reinterpret the data in a way that confirms their expectations.

These data support the identity-protective cognition thesis rather than the science-comprehension thesis. It seems unlikely that additional math and science training would help these folks to draw the correct conclusion when that conclusion conflicts with their ideology. One commentator referred to these results as “the most depressing discovery about the brain, ever.” (Journalists have a depressing tendency to refer to the results of psychological research as discoveries about “the brain,” falsely implying that they are physiologically determined. I would say these results demonstrate the effectiveness of political socialization.) They are consistent with findings showing that giving people information which corrects common misconceptions sometimes backfires and causes them to believe them more strongly. It also supplements Kahan's previous finding that the most scientifically literate Americans are not more convinced that climate change is a serious threat, but are more ideologically polarized than those who are less scientifically literate.

Despite the fact that the authors proposed that such results would raise doubts about the possibility of rational self-government, they do not recommend that we curtail participatory democracy and turn over our more important decisions to Big Brother. Kahan sees the problem as a failure of scientific communication. He proposes that scientific issues be reframed so as to reaffirm the ideological beliefs of those who might otherwise be skeptical. For example, in another article he suggests that global warming be reframed as a technical problem to be address by corporations rather than a political problem to be addressed by government:

They [conservatives] would probably look at the evidence more favorably, however, if made aware that possible responses to climate change include nuclear power and geoengineering. . . Similarily, [liberals] are less likely to reflexively dismiss the safety of nanotechnology if they are made aware of the part that nanotechnology might play in environmental protection, and not just its usefulness in the manufacture of consumer goods.

Here is a video of Kahan discussing the results of his global warming studies and his suggestions for improving scientific communication.


Kahan seems to be suggesting that our ideological disagreements can be solved through clever marketing, but this seems an unlikely solution. For example, any serious attempt to curtail climate change through either nuclear power or geoengineering would encounter strong and well-founded objections from important members of the scientific community, and consensus would quickly disappear.

Before we throw up our hands in despair, Kahan's results need to be replicated with a broader range of issues. However, to be fair, global warming is such an important issue that failure to reach consensus on a solution will make all the other issues which we think we care about seem like the equivalent of rearranging the deck chairs on the Titanic.

However, there are some research findings that seem to offer greater hope of overcoming ideological polarization. I will discuss them in future posts. But first, for those that are interested, an appendix about the differences between liberals and conservatives.

You may also be interested in reading:


Monday, October 14, 2013

Is Democracy Possible? Part 1

Let's not be naïve. Obviously, the political system we have now is more a plutocracy than a democracy. Our government has been captured by corporations and the people who own them. But suppose, by some miracle, we were able to enact effective campaign reform laws and politicians became responsive to public opinion. Are American citizens capable of making rational decisions based on scientific information?

Before we go any further, please take a look at this problem.

Table 1
The problem requires correctly interpreting a contingency table. Even bright college students often get it wrong. When deciding whether the skin cream works, some people only compare the numbers in the top row—the number of people who used the skin cream who get better or worse. But this ignores important information from the control group that didn't take the skin cream. Other people only compare the numbers in the left column—the number of people getting better who took the skin cream or didn't. But this ignores the fact that many more people took the skin cream than didn't. To correctly answer this question, you have to consider all four cells of the table. You must compare the percentage of people who used the skin cream who got better to the percentage of people who didn't use the skin cream who got better. Of the 298 people who used the skin cream, 223, or 75% of them, got better. Of the 128 people who didn't use it, 107, or 84% of them, got better. The correct answer is that the skin cream is ineffective.

Why is this problem so difficult? In part, it's because Dan Kahan and his colleagues—who did the study I'm about to report—put their thumb on the scales. They chose data such that, if you use either of the two shortcuts I mentioned—comparing only the numbers in the top row or the left column—you'll get the wrong answer. Fifty-nine percent of the participants in this study answered the problem incorrectly.

This problem illustrates the difference between System 1 and System 2 thinking, the central metaphor of this blog. According to Daniel Kahnemann, in his book Thinking, Fast and Slow, we have two cognitive systems. System 1 is automatic and effortless. We simply say and do what feels right. The System 1 response to this problem is to say that the skin cream works, because that's what it looks like at first glance. System 2 is deliberate and effortful. It comes into play when we take the time to analyze a situation carefully. It can be used to correct the errors that System 1 is prone to making. In this case, System 2 requires that you do the math.

As difficult as this problem is, it has one thing going for it. We don't have any emotional investment in whether this skin cream works. But what if the issue were one about which we had a prior hypothesis? In this case, System 1 could lead us to a second source of error, confirmatory biasthe tendency the interpret new information in a way that is consistent with our prior beliefs and ideological biases. Compare the problem in Table 1 to these three other problems.

Table 2
Does a city-wide ban on concealed weapons increase or decrease crime? Conservatives would probably expect crime to increase (since people can no longer use their concealed weapons to shoot bad guys), and would be resistant to information suggesting that gun control actually works. Of course, the opposite is true for liberals. Kahan expected political ideology to affect participants' responses to the gun control questions, but not the skin cream questions.

Now let's introduce another variable into the mix—numeracy. (I realize this is getting complicated, but please bear with me. It's worth it.) Numeracy—analogous to literacy—refers to mathematical competence and a tendency to use quantitative reasoning in appropriate ways. You would expect numerate people to do better when making judgments of contingency, at least when evaluating skin creams. But what happens when numerate people encounter data contradict that their political views?

We know that liberals and conservatives clash over scientific issues such as gun control and global warming. Kahan suggests two theories to explain these political impasses. The science-comprehension thesis suggests that people don't have enough training in science and math. With better education, people would be capable of correctly interpreting the results of empirical studies. The identity-protective cognition thesis is more pessimistic. It argues that our ideological polarization is so great that it cancels out even the ability of well-educated people to utilize their math and science skills. If the confirmatory bias trumps numeracy, Kahan believes that this raises serious questions about whether Americans are capable of enlightened self-government. We may have to turn over important decisions to committees of experts. (But who will choose the experts?)

What do you think? The answer provided by Kahan's research comes in Part 2.

Thursday, October 3, 2013

Doin' the Reminiscence Bump

The reminiscence bump is not a dance step, but a characteristic of autobiographical memoryour recall of our own life history. While you might expect memories to fade gradually with time, previous studies of older adults show better recall for events during their youth, between the ages of 10 and 30. One way to test this is with popular music. In studies in which participants listen to hit songs from before they were born to the present, they are more likely to recognize and prefer songs from their teens and twenties, with the bump peaking at age 23.5. Reminiscence bumps have also been found for films, books, sports figures, current events and personal memories.

It won't be a surprise to music fans that we show a lifelong preference for the songs of our youth. The audience for oldies shows consists mostly of people who were in high school or college at the time the music was originally popular.

Several explanations have been suggested for the bump. One possibility is that we had many vivid first-time experiences during late adolescence and young adulthood, which were encoded in memory more strongly due to their emotional content. The first experience of a given type may also become a prototype, which is more easily recalled than other members of the category. It has also been suggested that hormonal and neurobiological changes play a role.

In a new study by Krumhansl and Zupnick, 62 Cornell University students with an average age of 20 were played clips from the two top hits of every year from 1955 to 2009 in random order. They were asked whether they recognized each song, whether they liked it, and to rate its quality. They indicated whether they had personal memories of each song, and if so when and with whom they heard it. Here are the results for recognition, quality, liking and personal memories.


These students are too young to test for the reminiscence bump. Their better recall for songs released after 2000 has a trivial alternative explanation—that more recent songs are better remembered. However, there were “cascading reminiscence bumps”—two earlier, smaller bumps, which are seen most clearly in the recognition and personal memories data. One occurs at from 1980-1984, about the time their parents were 20, and the other in the '60s, the decade when most of their parents were born.

The obvious explanation for the 1980-84 bump is that as children and adolescents, we are exposed to out parents' favorite music and wind up liking some of it. (My parents were fans of big band music. I didn't care much for big bands when I lived at home, but I've gradually come to like them much better—a sleeper effect?) The authors suggest that the '60s bump could reflect the musical taste of the students' grandparents. They also entertain the hypothesis that '60s music is generally better known and of higher quality than the music of other decades—a claim I regard as suspect.

Krumhansl and Zupnick also asked their participants what genres of music they listened to while growing up and now. Of course, their sample was neither large nor representative of college students generally, but here are the results.


Neither jazz nor blues did well—a problem for the future of both genres. I was prepared to see them overshadowed by pop, rock and hip-hop, but they also did worse than classical, country and soundtracks! (Of course, in recent years, the more popular soundtracks have been collections of recent hits, rather than original music composed for the film.)

This article is cross-posted from my music blog, The Blues and the Abstract Truth.

You may also be interested in: