Cognitive psychologists point out that
the average person dramatically underestimates the malleability of
human memory. False memory syndrome refers to cases in which
people have an important memory that they truly believe, but which is
factually incorrect. Some people have been convicted of crimes they
did not commit, such as sexual abuse of children, on the basis of
memories “recovered” by alleged victims, usually with the aid of
mental health practitioners. Other innocent defendants have been
convicted of crimes to which they confessed under police interrogation, and of which they believed they were guilty, at least
for a time. A new study by Julia Shaw and Stephen Porter
provides some of the best evidence yet for the construction of
detailed and important false memories.
The authors used a paradign developed
by Elizabeth Loftus for her “lost in a shopping mall” study, in
which some college students were induced to develop this false
memory. After 126 Canadian students had volunteered to participate
in the study, their parents or primary caregivers were interviewed.
The purpose of the interview was to identify one highly emotional
event of a non-criminal nature that had occurred to the students
during their adolescence, and to ascertain that they had never been
arrested and charged with a crime. The first 60 students who met
these criteria participated in a series of three interviews, each
about one week apart.
In the first interview, they were asked
to recall and describe the true event and a false event allegedly
reported by their parents. In half the cases, the false event was
that they were arrested and charged with a crime—ten cases each of
assault, assault with a weapon, and theft. The other thirty false
events were non-criminal: an accidental injury, being attacked by a
dog, and losing a large amount of money. Since none of the
participants reported remembering the false event during the first
interview, they were given false contextual cues, were asked to try
to recover the memory, and were given advice as to how to recover it.
They were asked again to describe both events and answer several
questions about them during the second and third interviews. At the
end of the third interview, they were fully debriefed.
Participants were said to have accepted the false memory if they met two criteria: (1) they reported ten or
more details of the incident not suggested by the interviewer, and
(2) they reported during the debriefing that they truly believed the
event had occurred. Forty-four of sixty participants (73%) met these
criteria. The percentages did not differ very much among the six
types of false events.
The researchers were impressed not only
by the large number of participants who accepted the false memory but
also by their “richly detailed” descriptions of it. They
compared responses to the true and false memories among the 44
participants who experienced the false memory. These students
recalled more details of the true memory (92, on average) than the
false memory (72)—but 72 details is still fairly impressive for a
false memory. They had greater confidence in the true memory, and
experienced it as more vivid, but the amount of anxiety generated by
the true and false memories did not differ.
There are some important differences
between Brian Williams' false memory and those created in the Shaw
and Porter study. As far as we know, Williams' memory was not
prompted by anyone else. However, Loftus has suggested that he might
have been influenced by “audience tuning.” To paraphrase Jon
Stewart, audience responses to his tale might have excited the
pleasure centers in his brain and caused his story to drift toward
greater bravery over time. In addition, the memories created in the
study were not self-serving. Just the opposite. They remembered
being charged with a crime. Would their misremembering have been
even greater if the memory had been flattering to them?
Leslie Savan expressed her satisfaction
that at least someone is being punished for lying about our invasion
of Iraq, even if it's not one of the right people. Others
have spoken about the relative unimportance of Brian Williams'
battlefield exaggeration when compared to the many other lies he routinely told on behalf of the government and his corporate sponsors
every weekday night. In fact, we might speculate that NBC's main
reason for suspending him was not that he told a false story but his
loss of credibility, which might adversely affect public acceptance
of their nightly propaganda report.
Pittsburgh Post-Gazette
columnist Tony Norman reports that he was in the Monroeville Mall
last week when three people were shot, although he didn't see or hear
the incident. When he posted his experience on Twitter, one person
wryly commented, “in five years, he will say he was shot.”
By the way, did I ever tell you about
the time I rescued a dog from a burning building?
You may also be interested in reading:
Fantastic write-up. Thanks, Dr. Stires.
ReplyDelete