On July 31, Pittsburgh public safety director Stephen Bucar had an unpromising first day on the job. A week before, Allegheny County District Attorney Stephen Zappala sent him a letter requesting changes in the way the Pittsburgh city police conduct eyewitness identifications. Mr. Zappala said his office would no longer prosecute cases unless (1) the identification was made using a sequential rather than a simultaneous lineup and (2) the lineup was conducted by an officer not involved in the investigation. His demand may have been motivated by an abstract desire for justice, but it's more likely the result of two recent lawsuits filed by men who were mistakenly arrested based on faulty identifications using the simultaneous method.
But the Pittsburgh police, notorious
for their resistance to change, said something like, “That's not
how we do things around here.” Unfortunately, Mr. Bucar decided to back them up, citing what he believes to be conflict in
the scientific literature over whether sequential or simultaneous lineups are more likely to lead to error. This is disappointing news for
those of us who had hoped new Mayor Bill Peduto would bring more rationality to Pittsburgh government.
Stipulating that the identification
be conducted by an officer not involved in the case assumes that this
officer won't know which person in the lineup is the suspect. If so,
this double blind procedure prevents bias in which the
administrator unintentionally communicates the identity of the
suspect to the eyewitness. Since Mr. Bucar did not mention blind
testing in his statement, I presume he has no objection to it.
When an eyewitness views a lineup,
there are four possibilities. The culprit is either present in or
absent from the lineup, and the eyewitness either does or does not
make an identification. If the culprit is present and the eyewitness
correctly identifies the culprit, this is called a hit. When
the eyewitness either identifies someone else or makes no
identification, this is a miss. When the culprit is absent
and the eyewitness erroneously makes an identification, this is a
false alarm. Failure to identify anyone is a correct
rejection. Obviously, the goal is to maximize hits and correct
rejections, thereby minimizing misses and false alarms.
- Culprit PresentCulprit Absent
Identification HitFalse AlarmNo Identification MissCorrect Rejection
In the traditional simultaneous
procedure, the eyewitness is shown an array of (usually) six
photographs and asked which one, if any, is the perpetrator. In the
sequential procedure, the eyewitness is shown the photographs one at
a time, and is asked to state whether each one is the perpetrator.
In this procedure, eyewitnesses usually don't know how many photos
they will be shown.
In studies that compare these methods,
participants unexpectedly witness a simulated crime, either live or
on video, and are later asked to make an identification. In those
studies with complete designs, the perpetrator is present in
half the lineups, while in the other half, he or she is absent. Half
of each of these lineups are conducted using the simultaneous and the
sequential procedure. Since the researchers know whether the culprit
is present in each lineup, they can determine whether a correct
identification was made.
Several decades of research show the
sequential procedure to result in greater accuracy. In 2011, Nancy Steblay and her colleagues published a meta-analysis of all 72 known
studies (from 23 labs involving over 13,000 participants) comparing
simultaneous and sequential lineups. Simultaneous lineups produce
higher choosing rates (76%) than sequential lineups (61%), suggesting
than the simultaneous method encourages guessing. More importantly,
identifications from a sequential lineup contain fewer errors.
To illustrate this, here are the data from the 23 studies with complete designs (as described above).
Since all four conditions are present in these studies, it is
possible to compare their average results.
Hits from culprit-present lineups
Simultaneous 52%
Sequential 44%
Difference 8% fewer hits in
sequential
False alarms from culprit-absent
lineups
Simultaneous 54%
Sequential 32%
Difference 22% fewer false alarms
in sequential
There is a tradeoff. If all lineups
were culprit-present, the simultaneous method might appear to be
superior, since it produces 8% more identifications. However, since
the simultaneous method encourages guessing, some researchers argue
that this 8% difference should be treated as lucky guesses rather
than true identifications. When the culprit is absent, the
simultaneous method leads to 22% more false alarms. Therefore, the
sequential method is more accurate.
Why is the simultaneous method more
error prone? When an eyewitness is asked to view a lineup, he or she
probably infers that the police think they have arrested the
perpetrator. In a simultaneous array, eyewitnesses make a relative
judgment. They compare the photos to one another and choose the
one which most closely resembles their recollection of the
perpetrator. In the sequential method, however, because they don't know how many suspects they will be shown, they must make an absolute
judgment, comparing each suspect to their independent memory of
the perpetrator.
Here is a 61-min lecture by researcher Gary Wells
that I posted once before. Between minutes 14 and 30, he talks about
the relative judgment process and the need for sequential lineups.
You may also be interested in reading:
Eyewitness Blues, Part 2
Book Review: Failed Evidence, by David Harris
Another Miss By the Supremes
Eyewitness Blues, Part 2
Book Review: Failed Evidence, by David Harris
Another Miss By the Supremes
No comments:
Post a Comment
Comments are always welcome.