Monday, August 25, 2014

Eyewitness Blues, Part 1

The Issue

On July 31, Pittsburgh public safety director Stephen Bucar had an unpromising first day on the job. A week before, Allegheny County District Attorney Stephen Zappala sent him a letter requesting changes in the way the Pittsburgh city police conduct eyewitness identifications. Mr. Zappala said his office would no longer prosecute cases unless (1) the identification was made using a sequential rather than a simultaneous lineup and (2) the lineup was conducted by an officer not involved in the investigation. His demand may have been motivated by an abstract desire for justice, but it's more likely the result of two recent lawsuits filed by men who were mistakenly arrested based on faulty identifications using the simultaneous method.

But the Pittsburgh police, notorious for their resistance to change, said something like, “That's not how we do things around here.” Unfortunately, Mr. Bucar decided to back them up, citing what he believes to be conflict in the scientific literature over whether sequential or simultaneous lineups are more likely to lead to error. This is disappointing news for those of us who had hoped new Mayor Bill Peduto would bring more rationality to Pittsburgh government.

Stipulating that the identification be conducted by an officer not involved in the case assumes that this officer won't know which person in the lineup is the suspect. If so, this double blind procedure prevents bias in which the administrator unintentionally communicates the identity of the suspect to the eyewitness. Since Mr. Bucar did not mention blind testing in his statement, I presume he has no objection to it.

When an eyewitness views a lineup, there are four possibilities. The culprit is either present in or absent from the lineup, and the eyewitness either does or does not make an identification. If the culprit is present and the eyewitness correctly identifies the culprit, this is called a hit. When the eyewitness either identifies someone else or makes no identification, this is a miss. When the culprit is absent and the eyewitness erroneously makes an identification, this is a false alarm. Failure to identify anyone is a correct rejection. Obviously, the goal is to maximize hits and correct rejections, thereby minimizing misses and false alarms.
Culprit Present
Culprit Absent
Identification
Hit
False Alarm
No Identification
Miss
Correct Rejection
In the traditional simultaneous procedure, the eyewitness is shown an array of (usually) six photographs and asked which one, if any, is the perpetrator. In the sequential procedure, the eyewitness is shown the photographs one at a time, and is asked to state whether each one is the perpetrator. In this procedure, eyewitnesses usually don't know how many photos they will be shown.

In studies that compare these methods, participants unexpectedly witness a simulated crime, either live or on video, and are later asked to make an identification. In those studies with complete designs, the perpetrator is present in half the lineups, while in the other half, he or she is absent. Half of each of these lineups are conducted using the simultaneous and the sequential procedure. Since the researchers know whether the culprit is present in each lineup, they can determine whether a correct identification was made.

Several decades of research show the sequential procedure to result in greater accuracy. In 2011, Nancy Steblay and her colleagues published a meta-analysis of all 72 known studies (from 23 labs involving over 13,000 participants) comparing simultaneous and sequential lineups. Simultaneous lineups produce higher choosing rates (76%) than sequential lineups (61%), suggesting than the simultaneous method encourages guessing. More importantly, identifications from a sequential lineup contain fewer errors.

To illustrate this, here are the data from the 23 studies with complete designs (as described above). Since all four conditions are present in these studies, it is possible to compare their average results.

Hits from culprit-present lineups
          Simultaneous                     52%
          Sequential                         44%
          Difference                           8% fewer hits in sequential

False alarms from culprit-absent lineups
          Simultaneous                     54%
          Sequential                          32%
          Difference                          22% fewer false alarms in sequential

There is a tradeoff. If all lineups were culprit-present, the simultaneous method might appear to be superior, since it produces 8% more identifications. However, since the simultaneous method encourages guessing, some researchers argue that this 8% difference should be treated as lucky guesses rather than true identifications. When the culprit is absent, the simultaneous method leads to 22% more false alarms. Therefore, the sequential method is more accurate.

Why is the simultaneous method more error prone? When an eyewitness is asked to view a lineup, he or she probably infers that the police think they have arrested the perpetrator. In a simultaneous array, eyewitnesses make a relative judgment. They compare the photos to one another and choose the one which most closely resembles their recollection of the perpetrator. In the sequential method, however, because they don't know how many suspects they will be shown, they must make an absolute judgment, comparing each suspect to their independent memory of the perpetrator.

Here is a 61-min lecture by researcher Gary Wells that I posted once before. Between minutes 14 and 30, he talks about the relative judgment process and the need for sequential lineups.



No comments:

Post a Comment

Comments are always welcome.